Ted Cruz’s
Stone-Age Brain and Yours
Why “Collateral Damage” Elicits So Little Empathy
Among Americans
By Rick Shenkman
January 11, 2016 "Information
Clearing House"
-
"Tom
Dispatch" -
After Senator Ted Cruz suggested that the United
States begin carpet bombing Islamic State (IS)
forces in Syria, the reaction was swift. Hillary
Clinton
mocked candidates who use “bluster and bigotry.”
Jeb Bush
insisted the idea was “foolish.” Rich Lowry,
the editor of National Review,
tweeted: “You can't carpet bomb an insurgency
out of existence. This is just silly.”
When CNN’s
Wolf Blitzer
objected that Cruz’s proposal would lead to lots
of civilian casualties, the senator retorted
somewhat incoherently: "You would carpet bomb where
ISIS is -- not a city, but the location of the
troops. You use air power directed -- and you have
embedded special forces to direction the air power.
But the object isn't to level a city. The object is
to kill the ISIS terrorists." PolitiFact
drily
noted that Cruz apparently didn’t understand
what the process of carpet (or “saturation”) bombing
entails. By definition, it means bombing a wide area
regardless of the human cost.
By almost
any standard Cruz’s proposal was laughable and his
rivals and the media called him on it. What happened
next? By all rights after such a mixture of inanity
and ruthlessness, not to say bloody-mindedness
against civilian populations, his poll numbers
should have begun to sag. After all, he’d just
flunked the commander-in-chief test and what might
have seemed like a test of his humanity as well. In
fact, his poll numbers actually crept up. The week
before the imbroglio, an
ABC opinion poll had registered him at 15%
nationally. By the following week, he was
up to 18% and one
poll even had him at a resounding 24%.
How to
explain this? While many factors can affect a
candidate’s polling numbers, one uncomfortable
conclusion can’t be overlooked when it comes to
reactions to Cruz’s comments: by and large,
Americans don’t think or care much about the
real-world consequences of the unleashing of
American air power or that of our allies. The other
day, Human Rights Watch (HRW)
reported that, in September and October, a Saudi
Arabian coalition backed by the United States
“carried out at least six apparently unlawful
airstrikes in residential areas of the [Yemeni]
capital,” Sana’a. The attacks resulted in the
deaths of 60 civilians. Just about no one in the
United States took notice, nor was it given
significant media coverage. More than likely, this
is the first time you’ve heard about the HRW
findings.
You might
think that this is because the conflict in Yemen is
off our national radar screen. But how much
attention have Americans paid to U.S. air strikes
and bombing runs in Iraq? Washington has literally
been bombing Iraq on and off for twelve years and
yet few have taken much notice. That helps explain
why bombing is such an attractive option for
Washington any time trouble breaks out in the
world. Americans don’t seem to care much what goes
on when our bombs or missiles hit the ground. As
pollsters found recently, a surprising number of
Americans even
want to bomb places that can’t be found on a
map. When Public Policy Polling
asked GOP voters in mid-December if they
favored bombing Agrabah, 30%
said they did (as did 19% of Democrats), while only
13% opposed the idea. Agrabah is the fictional city
featured in the Disney movie Aladdin.
Would you
support or oppose bombing Agrabah?
Support bombing Agrabah…………………….. 30%
Oppose bombing Agrabah……………………… 13%
Not sure………………………………………………… 57%
That 57%
were “not sure” might be considered at least
modestly (but not wildly) reassuring.
Why
Cruz’s Numbers Went Up
History
suggests that this blanket bloodthirstiness or at
least lack of empathy for those on the other end of
America’s bombing campaigns isn’t new. In March
1951, nine months into the Korean War, Freda
Kirchwey, a crusading liberal journalist at the
Nation, expressed
bewilderment at American indifference to the
fate of Korean civilians killed by our bombs. The
destruction was awful. Little was left standing,
structurally speaking, in North Korea. Nothing, she
complained in a column, “excuses the terrible
shambles created up and down the Korean peninsula by
the American-led forces, by American planes raining
down napalm and fire bombs, and by heavy land and
naval artillery.” And yet few seemed bothered by it.
Because she
was an optimist Kirchwey expressed the hope that
Americans would eventually come to share her own
moral anguish at what was being done in their name.
They never did. If anything, the longer the war
ground on, the less Americans seemed interested in
the fate of the victims of our bombing.
Why did
they show so little empathy? Science helps provide
us with an answer and it’s a disturbing one: empathy
grows harder as distances -- whether of status,
geography, or both -- increase. Think of it as a
matter of our Stone Age brains. It’s hard because
in many circumstances an empathic response is, in
fact, an unnatural act. It is not natural, it turns
out, for us to feel empathy for those who look
different and speak a different language. It is not
natural for us to empathize with those who are
invisible to us, as most bombing victims were and
are. Nor is it natural for us to feel empathy for
people who have what social scientists call “low
status” in our eyes, as did the Korean peasants we
were killing. Recent
studies show that, faced with a choice of
killing a single individual to save the lives of
several people, we are far more apt to consider
doing it if the individual we are sacrificing is of
such low status. When subjects in an experiment are
told that high-status people are being saved, the
number willing to let the low-status victim die
actually increases.
Another
social science finding helps us understand why
empathy is often in short supply and why Ted Cruz is
capable of cavalierly recommending we carpet bomb
Syrians living under the control of the Islamic
State. Once we have convinced ourselves of the
necessity and correctness of bombing the hell out of
a country -- as Americans did during the Korean War
and as we are now doing in our war against IS -- the
wiring in our Stone Age brain helps us overcome any
hint of guilt we might be inclined to feel over the
ensuing loss of life. It quite naturally acts to
dehumanize the distant victims of our air strikes.
This is a
classic
case of cognitive dissonance. Our brain hates to
feel torn between conflicting emotions. Instead it
rationalizes doing what we want to do by discounting
any feeling that gives rise to negative emotions, in
this case, guilt. An extreme example of this was
what
happened when the Nazis decided to stigmatize
Jews and later wipe them out. From the moment they
began their ruthless anti-Semitic campaigns, they
used hideous imagery to convince other Germans that
Jews were not, like them, human at all, but little
different than rats. It is, of course, far easier
to kill someone, or to sit by while others do the
same, if you dehumanize them first. Rather than
feeling empathy for the downtrodden Jews, many
Germans felt contempt and disgust, strong emotions
that swamped whatever other feelings they might have
had.
In a
study a few years ago, researchers measured the
activity in the brains of subjects looking at
pictures of homeless people. The finding was
shocking. Brain activity in the medial prefrontal
cortex, the region of the brain where empathy is
often registered, was significantly lower than
normal. Put another way, the subjects in this
experiment literally paid the homeless no (or at
least less) mind.
This may
sound cruel and uncaring, but as far as biology is
concerned it makes sense. Our
genes, as the biologist Richard Dawkins has
taught us, are “selfish”; they are, that is, built
to enhance their own replication, which is, in
effect, their biological imperative. Caring for
people who are low in status, particularly those who
belong to another tribe, doesn’t serve this
imperative. Indeed, it may interfere with it by
diverting the attention of the host -- that’s you
and me -- from activities that will enhance our
survival.
Think of
this as our Stone-Age brains in action. It’s not
that we necessarily make a conscious decision to
ignore the fate of people who are low in status.
Our brain does this automatically and seamlessly for
us. Out of conscious awareness it decides if
someone is useful to us. If that person is, our
brain quickly achieves a state of
hyper-attentiveness: our nostrils flare, our eyes
widen, and our ears tune in relevant sounds.
Think of what happens when you’re in the
presence of somebody important and you’ll know what
I mean. If someone is deemed useless to us? Unless
we’re worried that they hyperpose a threat, our
brain tells our body to relax.
Because it
is in our biological interest to feel empathy for
people from our own tribe and family -- those, that
is, in a position to either enhance our survival or
perpetuate our genes -- we come equipped with
mechanisms to help us distinguish our people from
outsiders. From the moment we’re born, we focus on
those around us and bond with them. A mother and
child know each other through smell. Brother and
sister recognize each other’s familiar facial
features.
When we
hear someone speaking a foreign language, we
instinctively discount their humanity. This was
shown in a 2014
experiment designed to determine if human beings
were more willing to sacrifice someone who spoke a
different language in order to save the lives of
several others. The findings were clear-cut. Only
18% of the subjects in the experiment were willing
to make the cold calculation that saving the lives
of several people at the cost of one life was “fair”
when the intended victim shared their native
language. However, that percent more than doubled
when it was revealed that the person to be
sacrificed spoke a foreign language. The
experiment’s results remained the same whether that
language was Korean, Hebrew, Japanese, English, or
Spanish.
Why
Stories Matter When It Comes to American War
You may be
beginning to wonder if we aren’t doomed to eternal
indifference to the human beings who suffer when we
loose our Air Force on them, but science offers us a
modicum of hope on the subject. In recent years,
one of the strongest findings is that storytelling
can break through our indifference and foster
empathy even for distant peoples who might otherwise
seem alien to us. This more than anything else gives
us the ability to empathize with those with whom we
don’t identify demographically or otherwise. Stories
hold our attention, while feeding the strong urge to
find meaningful patterns in human behavior.
As
scientists have now
demonstrated in experiments, the brain is a
natural pattern finder. It wants one and one to
equal two. Mysterious may be the will of God, but
here on Earth we expect behavior to be explicable.
Stories are designed to establish cause and effect,
and once we understand what motivates people we can
usually find a way to empathize with them.
Stories
connect us to people in a way nothing else can.
It’s the reason politicians regularly tell stories
on the campaign trail. Years ago, Harvard social
scientist Howard Gardner
set out to discover what highly successful
leaders have in common. After reviewing the lives of
11 luminaries, from Margaret Thatcher to Martin
Luther King, Jr., he concluded that their success
depended to a remarkable extent on their ability to
communicate a compelling story or, as he put it,
“narratives that help individuals think about and
feel who they are, where they come from, and where
they are headed.” These stories, he found,
“constitute the single most powerful weapon in the
leader’s literary arsenal.”
When people
are reduced to numbers -- as were the civilian
victims of air power during the Korean War and as
are the civilians who become “collateral
damage” in American air strikes in Iraq, Syria,
and elsewhere -- we don’t feel their pain, nor do we
automatically put ourselves in their shoes, which is
by definition what you do when you are feeling
empathic. We have the bomber pilot’s syndrome. We
don’t feel anything for the victims below.
This is one
reason why antiwar movements matter. They tell
stories about the victims of war. It was striking
in the Vietnam years, for instance, how many
Americans came to care for, say, a small naked
Vietnamese girl
napalmed near her village, or so many other
Vietnamese civilians who
suffered under a rain of American bombs,
rockets, napalm, and artillery shells. The stories
that the massive antiwar movement regularly told
here about the distant world being decimated by the
U.S. war machine created a powerful sense of empathy
among many, including active-duty American soldiers
and veterans of the war, for the plight of the
Vietnamese. (It helped that few Americans believed
that North Vietnam posed an existential threat to
the United States. Fear brings out the worst in
us.)
Storytelling happens to be in every human’s
toolkit. We are all born storytellers and attentive
listeners. Biology may incline us to turn a cold
eye on the suffering of people we can’t see and
don’t know, but stories can liberate us. Ted Cruz
may be able to build up his poll numbers by
promising to carpet bomb foreigners in the Middle
East of whom we are fearful, but at least we know
that biology doesn’t have to dictate our response.
Our brains don’t have to stay in the Stone Age.
Stories can change us, if we start telling them.
Rick Shenkman is the editor and founder of the
History News
Network and the author most recently of
Political Animals: How Our Stone-Age Brain Gets in
the Way of Smart Politics
(Basic Books, January 2016).
Follow TomDispatch on
Twitter and join us on
Facebook. Check out the newest Dispatch Book,
Nick Turse’s Tomorrow’s
Battlefield: U.S. Proxy Wars and Secret Ops in
Africa, and Tom Engelhardt's latest book,
Shadow Government: Surveillance, Secret Wars, and a
Global Security State in a Single-Superpower World.
Copyright
2016 Rick Shenkman |